REFERENCES
Beauchamp G. 2010. Collective waves of sleep in Gulls (Larus
spp. ). Ethology, 117: 326-331.
Beauchamp G. 2015. Animal vigilance: Monitoring predators and
competitors. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Bednekoff PA. 2015. Sentinel behavior: a review and prospectus. Advances
in the Sstudy of Bbehavior, 47: 115-145.
Berger-Tal O, Polak T, Oron A, Lubin Y, Kotler BP, Saltz D. 2011.
Integrating animal behavior and conservation biology: a conceptual
framework. Behavioral Ecology, 22(2): 236-239.
Blumstein et al., eds. 2017. Ecotourism’s promise and peril: A
biological evaluation. Berlin: Springer.
Brandl SJ, Bellwood DR. 2015. Coordinated vigilance provides evidence
for direct reciprocity in coral reef fishes. Scientific Reports,
5:14556. doi: 10.1038/srep14556.
Caro TM. 2005. Antipredator defenses in birds and mammals. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Che Y, Yang L, Li ZQ. 2018. Vigilance synchronization of wintering
black-necked crane (Grus nigricollis ) families in Tibet.
Acta Ecologica Sinica, 38(4): 1375-1381.
Clutton-Brock, T. H., O’Riain, M. J., Brotherton, P. N. M.,Gaynor, D.,
Kansky, R., Griffin, A. S., Manser, M. 1999. Selfish sentinels in
cooperative mammals. Science, 284: 1640–1644.
Geffroy B, Samia DSM, Bessa E, Blumstein DT. 2015. How nature-based
tourism might increase prey vulnerability to predators. Trends in
Ecology and Evolution, 30(12): 755-765.
Harris J, Mirande C. 2013. A global overview of cranes: status, threats
and conservation priorities. Chinese Birds 4(3):189–209.
IPBES. 2019. Summary for policymakers of the global assessment report on
biodiversity and ecosystem services of the Intergovernmental
Science-Policy Platform on Biodiversity and Ecosystem Services. S. Díaz,
J. Settele, E. S. Brondízio E.S., H. T. Ngo, M. Guèze, J. Agard, A.
Arneth, P. Balvanera, K. A. Brauman, S. H. M. Butchart, K. M. A. Chan,
L. A. Garibaldi, K. Ichii, J. Liu, S. M. Subramanian, G. F. Midgley, P.
Miloslavich, Z. Molnár, D. Obura, A. Pfaff, S. Polasky, A. Purvis, J.
Razzaque, B. Reyers, R. Roy Chowdhury, Y. J. Shin, I. J.
Visseren-Hamakers, K. J. Willis, and C. N. Zayas (eds.). IPBES
secretariat, Bonn, Germany.
Kong DJ, Luo WX, Liu Q, Li ZQ, Huan GY, Zhang JJ, Yang XJ. 2018. Habitat
use, preference, and utilization distribution of two crane species
(Genus: Grus ) in Huize National Nature Reserve, Yunnan–Guizhou
Plateau, China. PeerJ, 6: e5105.
Kong DJ, Luo WX, Møller AP, Zhang YY, Yang XJ. 2020. Vigilance strategy
differentiation between sympatric threatened and common crane species.
Behavioural Processes, 176: 104119. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2020.104119.
Kong DJ, Yang XJ, Liu Q, Zhong XY, Yang JX. 2008. Diurnal time budget
and behavior rhythm of wintering black-necked crane (Grus
nigricollis ) at Dashanbao in Yunnan. Zoological Research
29(2):195–202.
Kong DJ, Zhong XY, Yang JX, Yang XJ. 2014. Seasonal and daily migration
behavior of Black-necked Cranes (Grus nigricollis ) in Yunnan,
China: timing and volume. Zoological Research, 35(S1): 181-188.
Li FS, Ma JZ. 1992. Wintering time budget and benefit study between
family and group Black-necked Cranes. Chinese Wildlife, 3: 36-41,29.
Li FS. 2014. IUCN Black-necked Crane (Grus nigricollis )
conservation plan. Zoological Research, 35(S1):3–9.
Li ZQ, Che Y, Yang L. 2017. Can sequential vigilance be predicted?
Behavioural Processes, 145: 81-85.
Liu Q, Yang XJ, Zhu JG, Zhao JL, Yu HZ. 2008. Flock of Black-necked
cranes wintering at Napahai nature reserve, China. Zoological Research,
29(5): 553-560.
Mainwaring MC, Griffith SC. 2013. Looking after your partner: sentinel
behaviour in a socially monogamous bird. PeerJ, 1:e83. doi:
10.7717/peerj.83.
Marion JL, Leung YF, Eagleston H, Burroughs K. 2016. A review and
synthesis of recreation ecology research findings on visitor impacts to
wilderness and protected natural areas. Journal of Forestry-Washington,
114(3): 352-362.
Pays O, Jarman PJ, Loisel P, Gerard JR. 2007a. Coordination,
independence or synchronization of individual vigilance in the eastern
grey kangaroo? Animal Bbehaviour, 73: 595-604.
Pays O, Renaud PC, Loisel P, Petit M, Gerard JF, Jarman PJ. 2007b. Prey
synchronize their vigilant behaviour with other group members.
Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 274: 1278-1291.
Podgórski T, Jong S, Bubnicki JW, Kuijper DPJ, Churski M, Jędrzejewska
B. Drivers of synchronized vigilance in wild boar groups. Behavioural
Ecology, 27(4): 1097-1103.
Pulliam HR. 1973. On the advantages of flocking. Journal of theoretical
Biology, 38: 419-422.
Quenette, P. Y., Gerard, J. F. 1992. From individual to collective
vigilance in wild boars (Sus scrofa ). Canadian Journal of
Zoology, 70, 1632-1635.
Rasa OAE. 1986. Coordinated vigilance in Dwarf Mongoose family groups:
the “Watchman’s song” Hypothesis and the costs of guarding. Ethology,
71: 340-344.
Rauber R, Manser MB. 2017. Discrete call types referring to predation
risk enhance the efficiency of the meerkat sentinel system. Scientific
Rereports, 7:44436. doi: 10.1038/srep44436.
Ridley AR, Nelson-Flower MJ, Thompson AM. 2013. Is sentinel behaviour
safe? An experimental investigation. Animal Behaviour, 85: 137-142.
Ridley AR, Raihani NJ, Bell MBV. 2010. Experimental evidence that
sentinel behaviour is affected by risk. Biology Letters, 6(4): 445-448.
Rodríguez-Gironés MA, Vásquez RA. 2002. Evolutionary stability of
vigilance coordination among social forages. Proceedings of the Royal
Society B, 269: 1803-1810.
Santema P, Clutton-Brock T. 2013. Meerkat helpers increase sentinel
behaviour and bipedal vigilance in the presence of pups. Animal
Behaviour, 85: 655-661.
Sirot E, Touzalin F. 2009. Coordination and synchronization of vigilance
in groups of prey: the role of collective detection and predators’
preference for stragglers. The American Naturalist, 173(1): 47-59.
Slater C, Cam G, Qi Y, Liu Y, Guay PJ, Weston MA. 2019. Camera shy?
Motivations, attitudes and beliefs of bird photographers and
species-specific avian responses to their activities. Biological
Conservation, 237: 327-337.
Steven R, Pickering C, Castley JG. 2011. A review of the impacts of
nature-based recreation on birds. Journal of Environmental Management,
92: 2287-2294.
Wang K, Yang XJ, Zhao JL, Yu HZ, Min L. 2009. Relations of daily
activity patterns to age and flock of wintering black-necked crane at
Napa Lake, Shangri-La in Yunnan. Zoological Research, 30(1): 74-82.
Ward PI. 1985. Why birds in flocks do not coordinate their vigilance
periods. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 114, 383-365.
Wickler W. 1985. Coordination of vigilance in bird groups: the
“watchman’s song” hypothesis. Z. Tierpsychol., 69: 250-253.
Wright J, Berg E, De Kort SR, Khazing V, Maklakov AA. 2001. Cooperative
sentinel behaviour in the Arabian babble. Animal Bbehaviour, 62:
973-979.
Xu, F., Ma, M., Yang, W.K., Blank, D., Ding, P., Zhang, T., 2013.
Vigilance in Black-necked Cranes: effects of predation vulnerability and
flock size. The Journal of Wilson Ornithology 125 (1), 208–212.
Zhang LX, An B, Shu ML, Yang XJ. 2017. Nest-site selection, reproductive
ecology and shifts within core-use areas of Black-necked Cranes at the
northern limit of the Tibetan Plateau. PeerJ 5: e2939. DOI
10.7717/peerj.2939.
TABLES AND FIGURES
TABLE 1. Vigilance (proportion and duration) difference between adults
and juveniles in three family groups of Grus nigricollis . The
first figure for each family type is the number of adult and the latter
is the number of juveniles in a family group. Bold figures indicate
significant level of p <0.05.