REFERENCES
Beauchamp G. 2010. Collective waves of sleep in Gulls (Larus spp. ). Ethology, 117: 326-331.
Beauchamp G. 2015. Animal vigilance: Monitoring predators and competitors. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Bednekoff PA. 2015. Sentinel behavior: a review and prospectus. Advances in the Sstudy of Bbehavior, 47: 115-145.
Berger-Tal O, Polak T, Oron A, Lubin Y, Kotler BP, Saltz D. 2011. Integrating animal behavior and conservation biology: a conceptual framework. Behavioral Ecology, 22(2): 236-239.
Blumstein et al., eds. 2017. Ecotourism’s promise and peril: A biological evaluation. Berlin: Springer.
Brandl SJ, Bellwood DR. 2015. Coordinated vigilance provides evidence for direct reciprocity in coral reef fishes. Scientific Reports, 5:14556. doi: 10.1038/srep14556.
Caro TM. 2005. Antipredator defenses in birds and mammals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Che Y, Yang L, Li ZQ. 2018. Vigilance synchronization of wintering black-necked crane (Grus nigricollis ) families in Tibet. Acta Ecologica Sinica, 38(4): 1375-1381.
Clutton-Brock, T. H., O’Riain, M. J., Brotherton, P. N. M.,Gaynor, D., Kansky, R., Griffin, A. S., Manser, M. 1999. Selfish sentinels in cooperative mammals. Science, 284: 1640–1644.
Geffroy B, Samia DSM, Bessa E, Blumstein DT. 2015. How nature-based tourism might increase prey vulnerability to predators. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 30(12): 755-765.
Harris J, Mirande C. 2013. A global overview of cranes: status, threats and conservation priorities. Chinese Birds 4(3):189–209.
IPBES. 2019. Summary for policymakers of the global assessment report on biodiversity and ecosystem services of the Intergovernmental Science-Policy Platform on Biodiversity and Ecosystem Services. S. Díaz, J. Settele, E. S. Brondízio E.S., H. T. Ngo, M. Guèze, J. Agard, A. Arneth, P. Balvanera, K. A. Brauman, S. H. M. Butchart, K. M. A. Chan, L. A. Garibaldi, K. Ichii, J. Liu, S. M. Subramanian, G. F. Midgley, P. Miloslavich, Z. Molnár, D. Obura, A. Pfaff, S. Polasky, A. Purvis, J. Razzaque, B. Reyers, R. Roy Chowdhury, Y. J. Shin, I. J. Visseren-Hamakers, K. J. Willis, and C. N. Zayas (eds.). IPBES secretariat, Bonn, Germany.
Kong DJ, Luo WX, Liu Q, Li ZQ, Huan GY, Zhang JJ, Yang XJ. 2018. Habitat use, preference, and utilization distribution of two crane species (Genus: Grus ) in Huize National Nature Reserve, Yunnan–Guizhou Plateau, China. PeerJ, 6: e5105.
Kong DJ, Luo WX, Møller AP, Zhang YY, Yang XJ. 2020. Vigilance strategy differentiation between sympatric threatened and common crane species. Behavioural Processes, 176: 104119. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2020.104119.
Kong DJ, Yang XJ, Liu Q, Zhong XY, Yang JX. 2008. Diurnal time budget and behavior rhythm of wintering black-necked crane (Grus nigricollis ) at Dashanbao in Yunnan. Zoological Research 29(2):195–202.
Kong DJ, Zhong XY, Yang JX, Yang XJ. 2014. Seasonal and daily migration behavior of Black-necked Cranes (Grus nigricollis ) in Yunnan, China: timing and volume. Zoological Research, 35(S1): 181-188.
Li FS, Ma JZ. 1992. Wintering time budget and benefit study between family and group Black-necked Cranes. Chinese Wildlife, 3: 36-41,29.
Li FS. 2014. IUCN Black-necked Crane (Grus nigricollis ) conservation plan. Zoological Research, 35(S1):3–9.
Li ZQ, Che Y, Yang L. 2017. Can sequential vigilance be predicted? Behavioural Processes, 145: 81-85.
Liu Q, Yang XJ, Zhu JG, Zhao JL, Yu HZ. 2008. Flock of Black-necked cranes wintering at Napahai nature reserve, China. Zoological Research, 29(5): 553-560.
Mainwaring MC, Griffith SC. 2013. Looking after your partner: sentinel behaviour in a socially monogamous bird. PeerJ, 1:e83. doi: 10.7717/peerj.83.
Marion JL, Leung YF, Eagleston H, Burroughs K. 2016. A review and synthesis of recreation ecology research findings on visitor impacts to wilderness and protected natural areas. Journal of Forestry-Washington, 114(3): 352-362.
Pays O, Jarman PJ, Loisel P, Gerard JR. 2007a. Coordination, independence or synchronization of individual vigilance in the eastern grey kangaroo? Animal Bbehaviour, 73: 595-604.
Pays O, Renaud PC, Loisel P, Petit M, Gerard JF, Jarman PJ. 2007b. Prey synchronize their vigilant behaviour with other group members. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 274: 1278-1291.
Podgórski T, Jong S, Bubnicki JW, Kuijper DPJ, Churski M, Jędrzejewska B. Drivers of synchronized vigilance in wild boar groups. Behavioural Ecology, 27(4): 1097-1103.
Pulliam HR. 1973. On the advantages of flocking. Journal of theoretical Biology, 38: 419-422.
Quenette, P. Y., Gerard, J. F. 1992. From individual to collective vigilance in wild boars (Sus scrofa ). Canadian Journal of Zoology, 70, 1632-1635.
Rasa OAE. 1986. Coordinated vigilance in Dwarf Mongoose family groups: the “Watchman’s song” Hypothesis and the costs of guarding. Ethology, 71: 340-344.
Rauber R, Manser MB. 2017. Discrete call types referring to predation risk enhance the efficiency of the meerkat sentinel system. Scientific Rereports, 7:44436. doi: 10.1038/srep44436.
Ridley AR, Nelson-Flower MJ, Thompson AM. 2013. Is sentinel behaviour safe? An experimental investigation. Animal Behaviour, 85: 137-142.
Ridley AR, Raihani NJ, Bell MBV. 2010. Experimental evidence that sentinel behaviour is affected by risk. Biology Letters, 6(4): 445-448.
Rodríguez-Gironés MA, Vásquez RA. 2002. Evolutionary stability of vigilance coordination among social forages. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 269: 1803-1810.
Santema P, Clutton-Brock T. 2013. Meerkat helpers increase sentinel behaviour and bipedal vigilance in the presence of pups. Animal Behaviour, 85: 655-661.
Sirot E, Touzalin F. 2009. Coordination and synchronization of vigilance in groups of prey: the role of collective detection and predators’ preference for stragglers. The American Naturalist, 173(1): 47-59.
Slater C, Cam G, Qi Y, Liu Y, Guay PJ, Weston MA. 2019. Camera shy? Motivations, attitudes and beliefs of bird photographers and species-specific avian responses to their activities. Biological Conservation, 237: 327-337.
Steven R, Pickering C, Castley JG. 2011. A review of the impacts of nature-based recreation on birds. Journal of Environmental Management, 92: 2287-2294.
Wang K, Yang XJ, Zhao JL, Yu HZ, Min L. 2009. Relations of daily activity patterns to age and flock of wintering black-necked crane at Napa Lake, Shangri-La in Yunnan. Zoological Research, 30(1): 74-82.
Ward PI. 1985. Why birds in flocks do not coordinate their vigilance periods. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 114, 383-365.
Wickler W. 1985. Coordination of vigilance in bird groups: the “watchman’s song” hypothesis. Z. Tierpsychol., 69: 250-253.
Wright J, Berg E, De Kort SR, Khazing V, Maklakov AA. 2001. Cooperative sentinel behaviour in the Arabian babble. Animal Bbehaviour, 62: 973-979.
Xu, F., Ma, M., Yang, W.K., Blank, D., Ding, P., Zhang, T., 2013. Vigilance in Black-necked Cranes: effects of predation vulnerability and flock size. The Journal of Wilson Ornithology 125 (1), 208–212.
Zhang LX, An B, Shu ML, Yang XJ. 2017. Nest-site selection, reproductive ecology and shifts within core-use areas of Black-necked Cranes at the northern limit of the Tibetan Plateau. PeerJ 5: e2939. DOI 10.7717/peerj.2939.
TABLES AND FIGURES
TABLE 1. Vigilance (proportion and duration) difference between adults and juveniles in three family groups of Grus nigricollis . The first figure for each family type is the number of adult and the latter is the number of juveniles in a family group. Bold figures indicate significant level of p <0.05.